Hot Hand

A cognitive social bias where an individual believes that a successful past performance can be used to predict success in future attempts

Over 1.8 million professionals use CFI to learn accounting, financial analysis, modeling and more. Start with a free account to explore 20+ always-free courses and hundreds of finance templates and cheat sheets.

What is Hot Hand?

Hot hand is a cognitive social bias where an individual believes that a successful past performance can be used to predict success in future attempts. People who believe in the hot hand phenomenon expect a trend to continue in the future. Such a line of thinking is not true since past results do not change the conditional independence of events occurring in the future.

Hot Hand

The hot hand and gambler’s fallacy are two important behavioral biases in financial markets that affect investment decisions. Hot hand belief is also common in sports, especially in basketball, where it is believed that when a player’s performance during a particular time period is significantly better based on his or her streaks of successful shots. The concept seems to affect the choice of players and the selection of the play.

Summary

  • The hot hand belief occurs when an individual erroneously assumes that a recent past results are a good predictor of future outcomes, when the events are independent of each other.
  • Investors are susceptible to the hot hand fallacy, and they will buy more funds or hold more diversified portfolios with the expectation of a continuous positive performance record.
  • Investment groups are less prone to the hot hand fallacy compared to individual investment decisions.

The Hot Hand Theory Explained

The hot hand fallacy theory was pioneered by Thomas Gilovich, Amos Tversky, and Robert Vallone in their famous “The Hot Hand in Basketball: On the Misperception of Random Sequences” (1985). The study, which investigated people’s intuitive conception of the belief in “hot hand” and “streak shooting,” drew from mathematical psychology, decision-making behaviors and heuristics, and cognitive psychology.

According to the study, outcomes of consecutive basketball shots are independent. The three men looked at the people’s inability to understand randomness and random events and how statistical information judgment triggers the wrong assumption regarding the random events.

Such kinds of thought patterns produced two related biases when applied to a coin toss. The first of the biases is the gambler’s fallacy, which makes an individual assume that a long sequence of either the head or tail increases the probability of getting a head or tail, respectively. Second, it leads to the rejection of randomness due to the belief that a streak of either outcome is non-representative. Economists refer to the hot hand concept as extrapolation bias.

Hot Hand in Investment Decisions

The hot hand belief is at play when investors think of buying and selling in the financial markets. The bias is common among investors who delegate decisions to professional fund managers. With the hot hand phenomenon, investors tend to purchase funds that were successful in the past, believing that the manager will prolong the performance record.

However, given the inconsistencies in fund performance, the hot hand fallacy may lead to a biased decision. Individuals with the fallacy believe that a particular person is hot and not a specific outcome. For example, investors tend to believe that if a professional manager purchased successful funds in the past, then whatever funds they settle on are likely to be profitable in the future.

A similar tendency is also manifest in lottery settings. The tendency of gamblers to redeem lottery tickets for more tickets instead of cash is consistent with the hot hand phenomenon; since individuals who enjoyed successive wins in the past believe that they are more likely to win again.

The hot hand belief emanates from illusion control, where people believe that they or others exert control over randomly determined events. Essentially, the hot hand fallacy surmises that, after a series of wins, investors will increase the number of shares they invest in and, after a loss, decrease them. It is done without factoring in the discounted future values of assets.

Hot Hand Belief in Individuals vs. Investment Groups

Investment groups are less prone to the hot hand fallacy and tend to decide more optimally than individuals in strategic and non-strategic settings. Even so, both groups and individuals alike exhibit hot hand beliefs. Investors who fall prey to such behavioral bias tend to hold less diversified portfolios, which can influence their risk exposure and, consequently, their expected returns. The behavioral biases that affect the investment decisions are based on the investment strategy, which differs between individuals and groups.

Investors who join investment groups can overcome their exposure to the hot hand fallacy. Under such an approach, amateur investors combine their investable holdings through a limited liability company or partnership in order to make a decision together and split the profits. Investment groups are different from mutual funds since, in the latter, shareholders hire professional fund managers to exercise rights on their behalf.

Purchasing and selling behaviors are also susceptible to the hot hand fallacy. The behavior can be seen when a customer misinterprets random market events and is influenced that a small sample represents the underlying process. Investors are more likely to purchase stock with positive trends in earnings. In the same manner, consumers are more likely to sell the stock with an adverse earning history.

Additional Resources

CFI is the official provider of the global Capital Markets & Securities Analyst (CMSA)™ certification program, designed to help anyone become a world-class financial analyst. To keep advancing your career, the additional resources below will be useful:

0 search results for ‘